Articles

Like a finger pointing at the moon… The Primacy of Direct Experience:

A Student Perspective  of The First International Symposium for Contemplative Studies

Sarah Wilson
Rex West, MA

Bhikshuni Lozang Trinlae, at right, describes her work on empirical studies of the effects of Vajrayana Buddhist practices.

How do we describe the ineffable? How do we make the non-dual tangible? How do we possibly quantify mindfulness? Over the weekend of April 26-29th, 2012, over 700 people attended the first International Symposia of Contemplative Studies (ISCS) in Denver, Colorado to explore the manifestations of these and many other important questions. Contemplative teachers, mindfulness researchers, educators, clinicians, doctors, and students joined together to share their work and consciously explore the burgeoning intersection of the fields of neuroscience and mindfulness. Keynote speakers at the conference included Jon Kabat-Zinn, Marsha Linehan (the founder of Dialectical Behavior Therapy), Richard Davidson (neuroscientist from the University of Wisconsin), and Ohio Congressman Tim Ryan.

As graduate students in counseling psychology at Naropa University in Boulder, this conference was incredibly eye-opening and transformational. Attending lectures on a variety of subjects, from caregiver burnout to the effects of mindfulness on vagal tone to mindfulness for treatment-resistant depression, our perspective was blown open in terms of the vast amount of academic exploration that is being conducted around the world on the benefits of mindfulness. Throughout graduate school, it has been easy for us to exist within the Naropa bubble, oblivious to the outside world and partially believing that the community of practitioners using mindfulness extends only a bit beyond the beautiful flatirons of Boulder. But the coming together of so many researchers and students of mindfulness demonstrated to us that this is in fact an emerging field, and the legitimacy of the field directly relates to the presence it has in mainstream research literature.

How do we bridge the worlds of quantitative research with the ineffable experiences of mindfulness meditation? How can we measure mindfulness? How can we measure the seemingly unmeasurable?   In this relatively new and groundbreaking field, this has come to be known as the “hard question.”   Researchers and practitioners from around the world have been exploring this question with the blessing of His Holiness the Dalai Lama, who has been bringing together neuroscientists and mindfulness practitioners to talk about their experiences measuring mindfulness since 1987 with the first Mind & Life dialogues in Dharamsala. The Dalai Lama himself believes that contemplative science is promising because it involves both the first person and third person phenomenological methods of examining experience.

Current Mind and Life president Arthur Zajonc, Mind and Life co-founder Adam Engle, and board member Jon Kabat-Zinn, who is at the University of Massachusetts Medical School Center for Mindfulness.

Many of these issues were addressed during the weekend at the ISCS.  “The International Symposium was a landmark meeting for Mind and Life,” said Mind and Life president Arthur Zajonc. “Beyond the superb science and contemplative scholarship, the conference brought together our whole community in a way that celebrated each person and their work. The conference demonstrated the power of the vision of Mind and Life to animate the imagination of scientists, contemplatives, and scholars alike in a common enterprise. The energy and excitement was palpable, and at its conclusion, many expressed to me their gratitude for the gathering and their impatience for the next.” Master lectures explored questions such as: what is meditation? What are we even referring to when we study the effects of meditation in a neuroscience laboratory? What is mindfulness?  What is compassion? While we throw around the terms mindfulness, compassion, and meditation as if to refer to specific delineated entities, in fact the diversity of these practices is vast.  Richard Davidson’s closing keynote discussed the critical nature of defining and describing these research terms in order to gain legitimacy for future research. In the pursuit of academic research rigor, he advocated bringing together transdisciplinary research teams to explore the questions of researching the effects of mindfulness from multiple angles, and many of the presenters encouraged the conference attendees to use the term mindfulness in their research more often, and define it clearly.

Given the focus on measurement, one could easily have become lost in the sea of statistics, fMRI images, and a plethora of graphs.  Luckily, in the majority of the sessions we attended the research was clearly translated for those of us without PhDs. While the conference clearly had a contemplative research focus, there were many sessions that were not focused primarily on the neuroscience yet more on the clinical applications of introducing mindfulness to specific populations in the therapeutic setting.  Marsha Linehan discussed the spiritual journey she has been on in founding Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT), a mindfulness-based therapeutic intervention and skills training originally developed for suicidal clients with Borderline tendencies. One concurrent panel session explored bringing mindfulness to difficult populations, including those with substance abuse issues, the homeless, the incarcerated, and clients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Another panel discussed the use of mindfulness with overweight clients in order to combat the growing obesity problem in the United States.

Anthony Chavez, Roshi Joan Halifax, and Brother David Steindl-Rast.

The weekend was a life-changing one, both in its eye-opening scope and the essential questions that it raised. Exploring the past, present and future of contemplative research, it was inspiring to be a part of such a large assembly of like-minded professionals, dedicated to using and legitimizing mindfulness in their respective fields. From a meditation practitioner’s standpoint, it was remarkable to learn from and sit next to dharma teachers like Roshi Joan Halifax, Matthieu Ricard, Sharon Salzberg, and Brother David Steindl-Rast. The opportunities for exploring and networking at this event were seemingly endless.  The Mind and Life Institute was so overwhelmed by the response to this first International Symposia for Contemplative Studies that they are currently planning to continue it biyearly.  For more information about the conference and to view lectures from the weekend, please visit www.contemplativeresearch.org.

Sidebar:  The Luminous Nature of Mind

One of the more enlightening and perplexing sessions was the Saturday evening keynote speech which was a “triologue” between Evan Thompson of the University of Toronto, Wolf Singer of the Max Planck Institute in Vienna, and Matthieu Ricard, a French-born Tibetean Buddhist Monk.  Ricard is well-known in the field of mindfulness study as he is the most often used guinea pig for formal studies of meditation and the mind.  This lively and heady panel was entitled, “The Nature of Consciousness:  Contemplative, Neuroscientific, and Philosophical Perspectives.”  What was really being considered here was not only the nature of consciousness, but consciousness itself; how, where, why, etc.  All three speakers spoke to the “hard question” of how experiences of consciousness intersect with the drive to measure and quantify consciousness.

Evan Thompson stated that, “Our ultimate task is not to solve an abstract mind-body problem, but to live this mutual dependence with mindfulness and compassion.”  Singer’s take is based in his search for what is known as the Neuronal Correlate of Consciousness (NCC), which is the proverbial needle in the haystack and Holy Grail of neuroscience.  It is theorized that here is where it is hoped lies the minimal set of neuronal events and mechanisms needed for a specific conscious percept or the brain’s understanding of subjective phenomena.  The conundrum of how the brain can truly conceive its own functioning has led Singer to consider whether there can exist consciousness without content.  In contrast to the Thompson and Singer, Matthieu Ricard approaches the question from his own first person experience of the nature of consciousness.  It is the luminous nature of the mind that is the common denominator of all mental events and brings us to the freshness of each present moment.  From his Buddhist perspective Matthieu stated that in the phenomenal world things do appear, yet the deeper we go, the more we realize the impossibility of trying to separate out our experience of consciousness from whatever may exist in the phenomenal world.  He likened this to the scientific search for smaller and smaller particles that make up all matter.  The pursuit of attempting to know all of the component parts gets in the way of us experiencing life.  In the end, they all seemed to agree that all of the progress neuroscience has made is exciting, but has not brought us any closer to finding consciousness in the brain.

photo credits:David Womack, Mind & Life Institute

Ritalin Gone Wrong

By L. ALAN SROUFE   Jan 28 2012 New York Times

THREE million children in this country take drugs for problems in focusing. Toward the end of last year, many of their parents were deeply alarmed because there was a shortage of drugs like Ritalin and Adderall that they considered absolutely essential to their children’s functioning.

But are these drugs really helping children? Should we really keep expanding the number of prescriptions filled?

In 30 years there has been a twentyfold increase in the consumption of drugs for attention-deficit disorder.

As a psychologist who has been studying the development of troubled children for more than 40 years, I believe we should be asking why we rely so heavily on these drugs.

Attention-deficit drugs increase concentration in the short term, which is why they work so well for college students cramming for exams. But when given to children over long periods of time, they neither improve school achievement nor reduce behavior problems. The drugs can also have serious side effects, including stunting growth.

Sadly, few physicians and parents seem to be aware of what we have been learning about the lack of effectiveness of these drugs.

What gets publicized are short-term results and studies on brain differences among children. Indeed, there are a number of incontrovertible facts that seem at first glance to support medication. It is because of this partial foundation in reality that the problem with the current approach to treating children has been so difficult to see.

Back in the 1960s I, like most psychologists, believed that children with difficulty concentrating were suffering from a brain problem of genetic or otherwise inborn origin. Just as Type I diabetics need insulin to correct problems with their inborn biochemistry, these children were believed to require attention-deficit drugs to correct theirs. It turns out, however, that there is little to no evidence to support this theory.

In 1973, I reviewed the literature on drug treatment of children for The New England Journal of Medicine. Dozens of well-controlled studies showed that these drugs immediately improved children’s performance on repetitive tasks requiring concentration and diligence. I had conducted one of these studies myself. Teachers and parents also reported improved behavior in almost every short-term study. This spurred an increase in drug treatment and led many to conclude that the “brain deficit” hypothesis had been confirmed.

But questions continued to be raised, especially concerning the drugs’ mechanism of action and the durability of effects. Ritalin and Adderall, a combination of dextroamphetamine and amphetamine, are stimulants. So why do they appear to calm children down? Some experts argued that because the brains of children with attention problems were different, the drugs had a mysterious paradoxical effect on them.

However, there really was no paradox. Versions of these drugs had been given to World War II radar operators to help them stay awake and focus on boring, repetitive tasks. And when we reviewed the literature on attention-deficit drugs again in 1990 we found that all children, whether they had attention problems or not, responded to stimulant drugs the same way. Moreover, while the drugs helped children settle down in class, they actually increased activity in the playground. Stimulants generally have the same effects for all children and adults. They enhance the ability to concentrate, especially on tasks that are not inherently interesting or when one is fatigued or bored, but they don’t improve broader learning abilities.

And just as in the many dieters who have used and abandoned similar drugs to lose weight, the effects of stimulants on children with attention problems fade after prolonged use. Some experts have argued that children with A.D.D. wouldn’t develop such tolerance because their brains were somehow different. But in fact, theloss of appetite and sleeplessness in children first prescribed attention-deficit drugs do fade, and, as we now know, so do the effects on behavior. They apparently develop a tolerance to the drug, and thus its efficacy disappears. Many parents who take their children off the drugs find that behavior worsens, which most likely confirms their belief that the drugs work. But the behavior worsens because the children’s bodies have become adapted to the drug. Adults may have similar reactions if they suddenly cut back on coffee, or stop smoking.

TO date, no study has found any long-term benefit of attention-deficit medication on academic performance, peer relationships or behavior problems, the very things we would most want to improve. Until recently, most studies of these drugs had not been properly randomized, and some of them had other methodological flaws.

But in 2009, findings were published from a well-controlled study that had been going on for more than a decade, and the results were very clear. The study randomly assigned almost 600 children with attention problems to four treatment conditions. Some received medication alone, some cognitive-behavior therapy alone, some medication plus therapy, and some were in a community-care control group that received no systematic treatment. At first this study suggested that medication, or medication plus therapy, produced the best results. However, after three years, these effects had faded, and by eight years there was no evidence that medication produced any academic or behavioral benefits.

Indeed, all of the treatment successes faded over time, although the study is continuing. Clearly, these children need a broader base of support than was offered in this medication study, support that begins earlier and lasts longer.

Nevertheless, findings in neuroscience are being used to prop up the argument for drugs to treat the hypothesized “inborn defect.” These studies show that children who receive an A.D.D. diagnosis have different patterns of neurotransmitters in their brains and other anomalies. While the technological sophistication of these studies may impress parents and nonprofessionals, they can be misleading. Of course the brains of children with behavior problems will show anomalies on brain scans. It could not be otherwise. Behavior and the brain are intertwined. Depression also waxes and wanes in many people, and as it does so, parallel changes in brain functioning occur, regardless of medication.

Many of the brain studies of children with A.D.D. involve examining participants while they are engaged in an attention task. If these children are not paying attention because of lack of motivation or an underdeveloped capacity to regulate their behavior, their brain scans are certain to be anomalous.

However brain functioning is measured, these studies tell us nothing about whether the observed anomalies were present at birth or whether they resulted from trauma, chronic stress or other early-childhood experiences. One of the most profound findings in behavioral neuroscience in recent years has been the clear evidence that the developing brain is shaped by experience.

It is certainly true that large numbers of children have problems with attention, self-regulation and behavior. But are these problems because of some aspect present at birth? Or are they caused by experiences in early childhood? These questions can be answered only by studying children and their surroundings from before birth through childhood and adolescence, as my colleagues at the University of Minnesota and I have been doing for decades.

Since 1975, we have followed 200 children who were born into poverty and were therefore more vulnerable to behavior problems. We enrolled their mothers during pregnancy, and over the course of their lives, we studied their relationships with their caregivers, teachers and peers. We followed their progress through school and their experiences in early adulthood. At regular intervals we measured their health, behavior, performance on intelligence tests and other characteristics.

By late adolescence, 50 percent of our sample qualified for some psychiatric diagnosis. Almost half displayed behavior problems at school on at least one occasion, and 24 percent dropped out by 12th grade; 14 percent met criteria for A.D.D. in either first or sixth grade.

Other large-scale epidemiological studies confirm such trends in the general population of disadvantaged children. Among all children, including all socioeconomic groups, the incidence of A.D.D. is estimated at 8 percent. What we found was that the environment of the child predicted development of A.D.D. problems. In stark contrast, measures of neurological anomalies at birth, I.Q. and infant temperament — including infant activity level — did not predict A.D.D.

Plenty of affluent children are also diagnosed with A.D.D. Behavior problems in children have many possible sources. Among them are family stresses like domestic violence, lack of social support from friends or relatives, chaotic living situations, including frequent moves, and, especially, patterns of parental intrusiveness that involve stimulation for which the baby is not prepared. For example, a 6-month-old baby is playing, and the parent picks it up quickly from behind and plunges it in the bath. Or a 3-year-old is becoming frustrated in solving a problem, and a parent taunts or ridicules. Such practices excessively stimulate and also compromise the child’s developing capacity for self-regulation.

Putting children on drugs does nothing to change the conditions that derail their development in the first place. Yet those conditions are receiving scant attention. Policy makers are so convinced that children with attention deficits have an organic disease that they have all but called off the search for a comprehensive understanding of the condition. The National Institute of Mental Health finances research aimed largely at physiological and brain components of A.D.D. While there is some research on other treatment approaches, very little is studied regarding the role of experience. Scientists, aware of this orientation, tend to submit only grants aimed at elucidating the biochemistry.

Thus, only one question is asked: are there aspects of brain functioning associated with childhood attention problems? The answer is always yes. Overlooked is the very real possibility that both the brain anomalies and the A.D.D. result from experience.

Our present course poses numerous risks. First, there will never be a single solution for all children with learning and behavior problems. While some smaller number may benefit from short-term drug treatment, large-scale, long-term treatment for millions of children is not the answer.

Second, the large-scale medication of children feeds into a societal view that all of life’s problems can be solved with a pill and gives millions of children the impression that there is something inherently defective in them.

Finally, the illusion that children’s behavior problems can be cured with drugs prevents us as a society from seeking the more complex solutions that will be necessary. Drugs get everyone — politicians, scientists, teachers and parents — off the hook. Everyone except the children, that is.

If drugs, which studies show work for four to eight weeks, are not the answer, what is? Many of these children have anxiety or depression; others are showing family stresses. We need to treat them as individuals.

As for shortages, they will continue to wax and wane. Because these drugs are habit forming, Congress decides how much can be produced. The number approved doesn’t keep pace with the tidal wave of prescriptions. By the end of this year, there will in all likelihood be another shortage, as we continue to rely on drugs that are not doing what so many well-meaning parents, therapists and teachers believe they are doing.

L. Alan Sroufe is a professor emeritus of psychology at the University of Minnesota’s Institute of Child Development.